With all due respect, I think you suffer from hardcore Dunning-Kruger when it comes to hearing research.
@Diesel has been relegated to a Kindergarten biology teacher so he has to simplify things, only to see it backfire.
Firstly, when he says that "increasing loudness is not what is supposed to happen at all," he is
NOT creating an upside down world and saying that "we would not want improved hearing thresholds." You read everything looking to see that narrative, no matter what.
What he
is saying is that when you regenerate OHC, the brain doesn't develop loudness hyperacusis (i.e. increase in central gain as a result of weakened input). If anything, it's the opposite — the central gain "loudness" is less because of restored input.
Put simply, OHC mechanically amplify low-level noises for the IHC to process. Then 95% of afferent neurons are Type I, which send hearing signal to the brain.
Let me say it loud and clear:
Everyone wants to see audiogram improvements. There's no "ah, it doesn't even matter, audiograms are useless" aspect. Of course, audiograms matter for hearing. In real world settings, they also matter for clarity because the volume of noise is not fixed and comfortable.
For example, if your friend whispers to you and you have audiogram problems, you may also have clarity issues making out what they are saying. The isolated role of IHC in clarity is totally different. The WR test
fixes the volume at a comfortable level. I know I've explained this to you before, but imagine your friend talking to you and you have no issues at all hearing them. But it just sounds like noisy, unclear garbage. This is the difference between "clarity problems" from IHC vs OHC.
If your friend talks and it feels like they were whispering and you say "repeat please," that's an OHC issue. If your friend shouts "cat" and you think they said "cap," that's more an IHC issue.
We all love audiograms. I would marry one if I could. God knows I'm lonely as fuck. But they aren't all there is to this thing.