Retigabine (Trobalt, Potiga) — General Discussion

Thanks for trying and helping the community Viking, I hope you get better soon, you deserve it, and don't forget the AUT00063 is around the corner, tables will turn at some point ;)
Thank you my friend. I hope so highly. I do not know what they are doing now but I think it will not take long to find out. I think up to 2 years! I hope! For now I have to take alfalitici and then come back to do experiments on the old tolep. A hug to everyone
 
Another update… went on a short 4 day vacation—drove quite a bit, took an extremely noisy ferry, ate in a bar/grill with live music on a patio (registered about 90db on my phone app)… and after all of that—no spike at all, no reactivity, the only times I heard tinnitus over the trip was every night before bed I would put my fingers in my ears to check if it was still there, two nights I heard a very faint whine and two nights I didn't hear anything
 
@Mpt - I have a question about the ferry you took.

Did you happen to measure it on your dB App? (if not how loud do u estimate it?)
Was the noise loud inside the cabin as well -or only on deck?
Where you using hearing protection on board?
 
@benryu , just wanted to say I enjoy very much reading your posts :bookworm:

Interesting what you said about glutamate inhibiting proper potassium channel function - is that a documented phenomenon?
Where exactly is this process happening?

Yeah a shit load of articles exist on the topic:
http://oto.sagepub.com/content/149/2_suppl/P94.1.abstract (directly related to the AM101)
http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2012.00009/full#B22 (animal model, super interesting)
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1359644605035427 (oldish article but very interesting, not sure if you can access it you don't have a University VPN access)
http://www.findthecurenow.org/a-personal-message/ (also they mention it in official organization such a the Canadian Tinnitus Foundation)

I could find probably another 10 or 20 articles explaining the phenomenon. :)
 
Another update… went on a short 4 day vacation—drove quite a bit, took an extremely noisy ferry, ate in a bar/grill with live music on a patio (registered about 90db on my phone app)… and after all of that—no spike at all, no reactivity, the only times I heard tinnitus over the trip was every night before bed I would put my fingers in my ears to check if it was still there, two nights I heard a very faint whine and two nights I didn't hear anything
Hell yeah you goooo boy!!! ;)
 
Sorry for incisting, but there is no concrete source demonstrating what you said, please share a link or somethig, the AM-101 is basically a glutamate blocker that tries to prevent the glutamate to affect potassium channels.

Acoustic trauma -> hair cells die -> they release glutamate (I simplify here)-> the glutamate turn the potassium channels in a low activity state in the freaking auditory cortex-> No potassium to control excitability = tinnitus

It doesn't move from the auditory cortext, imagine it would go to Gustatory cortex, you could taste T. ! I am pretty sure it would taste pretty bad tho.

Please guys stop with this weird idea, the T. is going nowhere, time only matters when hair cells die as glutamate is being released, for the rest it doesn't move.
Hmmm you make this seem soo easy to understand. So basically all that theory about T being "stuck" in the brain after some time(some say 3 monthes, 6 monthes, 1 year until its considered chronic) which is bs BUT that it only after some time can effect the pottassium channels making T permanent but with drug like autifony, then this is deffinally the good route to go cause if the T does end up effecting the pottasium channels like all of us here are definally effected.. Then a drug to treat the pottasium channels again such as autifony.. Then it will normalize it out again and T will fadeeeeee! Was that a good summary of what im understanding here? Lol seems like alot of docs out there are just pickin up on these pottasium channels being the reason for chronic T.. And some still are stupid to believe/understand it. Anyways thanks for explaining all of that! :)
 
Yeah a shit load of articles exist on the topic:
http://oto.sagepub.com/content/149/2_suppl/P94.1.abstract (directly related to the AM101)
http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2012.00009/full#B22 (animal model, super interesting)
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1359644605035427 (oldish article but very interesting, not sure if you can access it you don't have a University VPN access)
http://www.findthecurenow.org/a-personal-message/ (also they mention it in official organization such a the Canadian Tinnitus Foundation)

I could find probably another 10 or 20 articles explaining the phenomenon. :)
Thanks benryu for all your good information here. I do have questions since you know so much about brain chemistry, overactive neurons, potassium channels etc.
- Is there any way to reduce this overactivity by for example supplements?
- What do you do to cope with your T?
- Has masking or any sound therapy any effect on those overactive neurons?
- Why is T sometimes louder, sometimes quieter? Are the neurons firing in different strength?
- So is there any way at all reducing this on the long run by any treatment?
- Or is it what it is and we have to get used to it - until something stops this brain disorder?
Thanks in advance.
 
Another update… went on a short 4 day vacation—drove quite a bit, took an extremely noisy ferry, ate in a bar/grill with live music on a patio (registered about 90db on my phone app)… and after all of that—no spike at all, no reactivity, the only times I heard tinnitus over the trip was every night before bed I would put my fingers in my ears to check if it was still there, two nights I heard a very faint whine and two nights I didn't hear anything

Anyway, you have to be very carefull and have to be prepared to use earplugs. Dont unterestimate protection of your damaged hearing.
 
I've said this again and again. Researchers and people that claim that tinnitus is somehow memorized by the brain or is in a permanent state are just copping out because they don't want to or can't explain things further. If that was the case, why are there literally millions of cases of chronic tinnitus getting worse?

If something can degrade more, then that means it's not memorized or permanent or above plasticity in some way.

@benryu has explained it wonderfully.
 
Yeah a shit load of articles exist on the topic:
http://oto.sagepub.com/content/149/2_suppl/P94.1.abstract (directly related to the AM101)
http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2012.00009/full#B22 (animal model, super interesting)
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1359644605035427 (oldish article but very interesting, not sure if you can access it you don't have a University VPN access)
http://www.findthecurenow.org/a-personal-message/ (also they mention it in official organization such a the Canadian Tinnitus Foundation)

I could find probably another 10 or 20 articles explaining the phenomenon. :)
One link says drink more beer! Haha
 
Thanks benryu for all your good information here. I do have questions since you know so much about brain chemistry, overactive neurons, potassium channels etc.
- Is there any way to reduce this overactivity by for example supplements?
- What do you do to cope with your T?
- Has masking or any sound therapy any effect on those overactive neurons?
- Why is T sometimes louder, sometimes quieter? Are the neurons firing in different strength?
- So is there any way at all reducing this on the long run by any treatment?
- Or is it what it is and we have to get used to it - until something stops this brain disorder?
Thanks in advance.

- Is there any way to reduce this overactivity by for example supplements?
Yes but it's a case by case, for me I reduced sodium in my alimentation as well as carbs, reducing carbs helped me a lot :p . It's quite logical when you think how insulin and potassium channels are liked.

- What do you do to cope with your T?
Perspective & framing is important, imagine someone put your head underwater with no notice or reason. You are going to panic.
Now same thing, a friend tells you that he is going to put your head underwater for 5 sec then he will stop.
Still not really cool but you know it will be over soon.
This is the same for T. most people have their head underwater but don't realize, with science progress, it's for a relatively short time. Plus T. doesn't change who you are or what you can do, a little sound is not going to define your life.
I love poetry a lot, and there is one verse of Invictus by Henley that is a good illustration of this idea:

"It matters not how strait the gate,
How charged with punishments the scroll.
I am the master of my fate:
I am the captain of my soul."

http://www.poemhunter.com/poem/invictus/

- Has masking or any sound therapy any effect on those overactive neurons?
Masking help you to have rest and reduce the excitability in the limbic region of the brain. It doesn't have lasting effect but helps the mood a lot.

- Why is T sometimes louder, sometimes quieter? Are the neurons firing in different strength?
Exactly, it depends on many factors food, fatigue, ambiant noise, stress, etc...

- So is there any way at all reducing this on the long run by any treatment?
Several medicine help to cope with the T. but don't necessarely cure it yet. Several real cures are being tested and will very help us very soon. It's really near.

- Or is it what it is and we have to get used to it - until something stops this brain disorder?
I would not say that you have to get use to it, because you won't have it all your life. I guarantee it.
But you should accomodate to your T. while the cure is brough to market.
 
@benryu

Sorry if I missed it (it's a long thread) but could you please explain the link between potassium and insulin?

My insulin is higher than normal (fasting) and it's all due to t related stress, lack of sleep, crap diet, no exercise, so I'm just curious to see if reducing carbs would help.

I guess I will have to try:)

Thanks!
 
@benryu

Sorry if I missed it (it's a long thread) but could you please explain the link between potassium and insulin?

My insulin is higher than normal (fasting) and it's all due to t related stress, lack of sleep, crap diet, no exercise, so I'm just curious to see if reducing carbs would help.

I guess I will have to try:)

Thanks!

I'll try to keep it as simple as possible because it's pretty complicated.

Insulin is an hormone produced by the pancreas. It regulates the metabolism of carbohydrates and fats. It promotes the absorption of glucose from the blood to skeletal muscles and fat tissue.

The glucose that goes in the bloodstream after food consumption gets converted to Glucose-6-Phosphate through Glucokinase. This shit oxidized to form ATP (Adenosine triphosphate). This process inhibits the ATP sensitive potassium ion channels of the cell causing the Potassium ion channel to close and not function anymore. The closure of the Potassium channels causes Depolarization of the cell membrane causing the cell membrane to stretch which causes the voltage-gated.

So in simple terms, if you eat sugar, potatoe, pasta, rice or stuff with high carbs, your pancrea gonna release a lot of insulin that will take the glucose and mess with the potassium channels.
For healthy poeple it's not a big deal but for people with some potassium channel disorders (Seizure, Tinnitus, Justin Bieber Fans), if low performing potassium channels are slowed down even more in the process, then it will increase symptom.
Not absorbing glucose or low glucose level, will help the potassium channels to stay open.


Source:
What's insulin ?
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Insulin
What's ATP?
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Adenosine_triphosphate
What's the insulin signal transduction pathway and regulation of blood glucose?
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Insulin_signal_transduction_pathway_and_regulation_of_blood_glucose
Example of ketogenic diet on seizure:
http://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/05/unraveling-the-secrets-of-the-epilepsy-diet/
http://epilepsy.med.nyu.edu/diagnosis-treatment/diet/modified-atkins-diet#sthash.t9IPFL9l.dpbs
Low carbs diet and tinnitus:
http://www.tinnitusformula.com/library/sugar-metabolism-affects-tinnitus/#.U-FfEvl5N8E
(not super scientific :p )
 
@benryu
Thank you so much for your reply, will try to cut the curbs to see if it makes any difference.
Man with all your knowledge you should be on the research team to find a cure for this, you know more than most doctors or specialists!
 
Hi guys,I am another retigabine guinea pig.Initially I wanted to write only about long term results but now I have a problem: I'm taking retigabine since the beginning of this month,I started with 100 mg/day and then increased to 200 mg/day after two days.Now I think I'm having a very slight vision loss...nothing extreme but it seems I have more difficulties reading distant letters,like car plates.It's very strange cause I'm on a low dosage and It's difficult to quantify or conferm this problem because it's slight.I'm not having positive effects on tinnitus but I don't want to discontinue this drug because my tinnitus progressively increases and I really need to find a solution to manage it.What can I do?
P.S. I'm a @Viking 's friend so he can legitimate my story.
Greetings.
 
So sorry to hear that. Wish you a speedy recovery, friend.

Dear friends, today I did the ultrasound to the kidneys and bladder, unfortunately I have kidney problems (calcification and kidney stones) and I have the right to discontinue the drug. The doctor who examined me has banned the use of the drug by saying that I have serious health problems. I regret immensely. He was working for a while. I hope for the best for you. God bless you
Ivan
 
Hi guys,I am another retigabine guinea pig.Initially I wanted to write only about long term results but now I have a problem: I'm taking retigabine since the beginning of this month,I started with 100 mg/day and then increased to 200 mg/day after two days.Now I think I'm having a very slight vision loss...nothing extreme but it seems I have more difficulties reading distant letters,like car plates.It's very strange cause I'm on a low dosage and It's difficult to quantify or conferm this problem because it's slight.I'm not having positive effects on tinnitus but I don't want to discontinue this drug because my tinnitus progressively increases and I really need to find a solution to manage it.What can I do?
P.S. I'm a @Viking 's friend so he can legitimate my story.
Greetings.
How long have you had your tinnitus? Sorry to hear about the side effects. Hopefully you'll see some positives soon. Welcome to TT!
 
What can I do?

Continue to take the drug. The side effects you describe would be temporary ones - and these may, or may not improve over time. So the best option is to continue and see what happens (as long as the side effects are tolerable, of course).

Only after long-term usage will (some of) the side effects become permanent.
 
In 3 days i will end my Retigabine, I dont have more pills, this was experimental dosage.

Cognitive effect
I was harder to speak, to walk, you needed to concentrate when you type calculate, speak, often in speak you say work with letter missing, groggy like drunk state. Those effect start after 1h from taking and they are mostly serious in from onset 1,5h . They seem to get less intrusive as longer you use therapy.

I have tried 300 mg on 2 days and returned to 200 mg. 200 mg helped me feel better than 300, maybe i did not took long time 200 to test 300.

Tinnitus had several times totally died into silence on short period of time some 15 seconds. I noticed that spike did not appear any more, and t went lower. I did not know how much lower until i now started getting of, it was 30% lower.

My honest way how did i felt i will say BETTER, I went out much more, I smiled, talkative, worked in garden with father, i been here and there insisting people who I avoided at all cost. It gave me 2 weeks of life even than t was still existing, I slept better even that there was t there but he was as i say noticeable when i look/search for him, other times 70% or time i did not pay attention on him, he was habituated, there were some bad days BUT not like before, they were PART of days, morning or afternoon, never evening, and so louder t mostly because i eat 150 mg milk chocolate (that i could not touch before) i would get few hours of bad tinnitus but survivable.

Anyway i had positive effect, no allergy, no problem or any change of urine color, nothing there
 
Yeah a shit load of articles exist on the topic:
http://oto.sagepub.com/content/149/2_suppl/P94.1.abstract (directly related to the AM101)
http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2012.00009/full#B22 (animal model, super interesting)
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1359644605035427 (oldish article but very interesting, not sure if you can access it you don't have a University VPN access)
http://www.findthecurenow.org/a-personal-message/ (also they mention it in official organization such a the Canadian Tinnitus Foundation)

I could find probably another 10 or 20 articles explaining the phenomenon. :)

Not to be a stick in the mud, but there isn't one word about potassium channels in these articles. I'm looking for something that mentions anything about glutamate affecting K channels.
 
Dear friends, today I did the ultrasound to the kidneys and bladder, unfortunately I have kidney problems (calcification and kidney stones) and I have the right to discontinue the drug. The doctor who examined me has banned the use of the drug by saying that I have serious health problems. I regret immensely. He was working for a while. I hope for the best for you. God bless you
Ivan

Retigabine can cause kidney stones, but anyway try to see about breaking up of kidney stones and dring much more water than you drink now, take 2 litters.

When stone start roiling it can be painful, try to find smasmolitic medicine (Trospium chloride, Spazmex, Sanctura) something to relax tubes, and of coure jumping with a lot water. To make water running take 1 dl every 10 minutes until kiney start just trowing out water , white urine, then jumping mostly if you have building or a lot of steps warm beer and mineral water and jumping down the stairs, so there is vibration you feel and stones get out in 2-3 h, ol later in evening you pee it out, warm beer 1-2 and mineral water (the one wth Co2) and just jump it out and finished.
 
Retigabine can cause kidney stones, but anyway try to see about breaking up of kidney stones and dring much more water than you drink now, take 2 litters.

When stone start roiling it can be painful, try to find smasmolitic medicine (Trospium chloride, Spazmex, Sanctura) something to relax tubes, and of coure jumping with a lot water. To make water running take 1 dl every 10 minutes until kiney start just trowing out water , white urine, then jumping mostly if you have building or a lot of steps warm beer and mineral water and jumping down the stairs, so there is vibration you feel and stones get out in 2-3 h, ol later in evening you pee it out, warm beer 1-2 and mineral water (the one wth Co2) and just jump it out and finished.
A clarification. My kidney stones are not due to the use of Trobalt but they were already there! The doctor has advised me not to continue treatment because the drug can exacerbate this situation. Thanks to all
 
Hi guys,I am another retigabine guinea pig.Initially I wanted to write only about long term results but now I have a problem: I'm taking retigabine since the beginning of this month,I started with 100 mg/day and then increased to 200 mg/day after two days.Now I think I'm having a very slight vision loss...nothing extreme but it seems I have more difficulties reading distant letters,like car plates.It's very strange cause I'm on a low dosage and It's difficult to quantify or conferm this problem because it's slight.I'm not having positive effects on tinnitus but I don't want to discontinue this drug because my tinnitus progressively increases and I really need to find a solution to manage it.What can I do?
P.S. I'm a @Viking 's friend so he can legitimate my story.
Greetings.
Cheer up my friend! Do not give up! The blurred vision is only momentary. Even happened to me. Do not worry!
 
@Mpt - I have a question about the ferry you took.

Did you happen to measure it on your dB App? (if not how loud do u estimate it?)
Was the noise loud inside the cabin as well -or only on deck?
Where you using hearing protection on board?

Hey Dan,

I didn't record it on my decibel meter app. I would estimate it was 85-90db, and I didn't use hearing protection. The ride took about 20 minutes each way. It was a two deck ferry and we were sitting on the top which was uncovered, it was actually less loud up there as it was down below.

-Matt
 
Continue to take the drug. The side effects you describe would be temporary ones - and these may, or may not improve over time. So the best option is to continue and see what happens (as long as the side effects are tolerable, of course).

Only after long-term usage will (some of) the side effects become permanent.

initially my thinking was the same but after 3 days I'm starting to be worried
 
Not to be a stick in the mud, but there isn't one word about potassium channels in these articles. I'm looking for something that mentions anything about glutamate affecting K channels.

No problem, legit question, it's in the second article, but I guess you cannot access it without a University account.

So you can read more about this phenomenon simply on free reressources:

(It introduces the concept of excitotoxicity)
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Glutamate_receptor

(Several articles show how excitoxicity mess up potassium channels)
http://etd.fcla.edu/CF/CFE0001911/Soundarapandian_Mangala-Meenakshi_200712_PhD.PDF.pdf
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12766083

(this one is not totally related, but it explains well some points)
http://www.cochlea.eu/en/pathology/surdites-neuro-sensorielles/excitotoxicite

(Oldish but excellent textbooks that connects a lot of things, some of staments were contradicted by recent discoveries but overall it's nice :) )
http://books.google.ca/books?id=YStcWFsxQZEC&pg=PA262&lpg=PA262&dq=excitotoxicity tinnitus potassium&source=bl&ots=hBF_b1hSDL&sig=idGsse-9BDpMTBhTbCEUC7grnSo&hl=en&sa=X&ei=WTPiU_G0BsqJogSG74K4Ag&ved=0CDsQ6AEwAw#v=onepage&q=excitotoxicity tinnitus potassium&f=false

http://books.google.ca/books?id=XydUtbVHtdEC&pg=PA312&lpg=PA312&dq=glutamate potassium channel tinnitus&source=bl&ots=QlZNDeZwKY&sig=4VpEmCI6LwW9bp2_WYa8t9BkSGI&hl=en&sa=X&ei=RTXiU8jqCJKgogTfpoDYCw&ved=0CEMQ6AEwBA#v=onepage&q=glutamate potassium channel tinnitus&f=false
 
@benryu - did you mean this article? (I dont see K channels). I did see mention on Katp channel involved in another article you linked - are those channels related to Kv3 channels?
P.S. in this article they are opposing Dr.Llinas/Jeanmonod's hypothesis.

Gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamic acid levels in the auditory pathway of rats with chronic tinnitus: a direct determination using high resolution point-resolved proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS)

Thomas Brozoski1*, Boris Odintsov2 and Carol Bauer1
  • 1Division of Otolaryngology, Southern Illinois University School of Medicine, Springfield, IL, USA
  • 2Biomedical Imaging Center, Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois, Urbana Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
Damage to the auditory system following high-level sound exposure reduces afferent input. Homeostatic mechanisms appear to compensate for the loss. Overcompensation may produce the sensation of sound without an objective physical correlate, i.e., tinnitus. Several potential compensatory neural processes have been identified, such as increased spontaneous activity. The cellular mechanisms enabling such compensatory processes may involve down-regulation of inhibitory neurotransmission mediated by Γ-amino butyric acid (GABA), and/or up-regulation of excitatory neurotransmission, mediated by glutamic acid (Glu). Because central processing systems are integrated and well-regulated, compensatory changes in one system may produce reactive changes in others. Some or all may be relevant to tinnitus. To examine the roles of GABA and Glu in tinnitus, high resolution point-resolved proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) was used to quantify their levels in the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), inferior colliculus (IC), medial geniculate body (MGB), and primary auditory cortex (A1) of rats. Chronic tinnitus was produced by a single high-level unilateral exposure to noise, and was measured using a psychophysical procedure sensitive to tinnitus. Decreased GABA levels were evident only in the MGB, with the greatest decrease, relative to unexposed controls, obtained in the contralateral MGB. Small GABA increases may have been present bilaterally in A1 and in the contralateral DCN. Although Glu levels showed considerable variation, Glu was moderately and bilaterally elevated both in the DCN and in A1. In the MGB Glu was increased ipsilaterally but decreased contralaterally. These bidirectional and region-specific alterations in GABA and Glu may reflect large-scale changes in inhibitory and excitatory equilibrium accompanying chronic tinnitus. The present results also suggest that targeting both neurotransmitter systems may be optimal in developing more effective therapeutics.

Introduction
Chronic tinnitus affects a large number of people, with perhaps 3–5 percent of the tinnitus population significantly disturbed by their condition. Exposure to damaging high-level sound is likely the leading cause of tinnitus in young people (Muhr and Rosenhall, 2011) and the second leading cause in older adults (Nondahl et al., 2002). Damage to the peripheral auditory system, in the long-term, reduces afferent input to the central auditory system and appears to stimulate compensatory central changes. The compensation, or perhaps overcompensation, may produce the sensation of sound where none exists, i.e., tinnitus. The neural correlates of tinnitus identified in animal studies are summarized in Table 1. Cellular mechanisms responsible for these changes have been hypothesized to include a down-regulation of inhibitory neurotransmission, such as that mediated by the neurotransmitter Γ-amino butyric acid (GABA) and/or an up-regulation of excitatory neurotransmission, such as that mediated by the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate (Glu). Can alterations in the concentration of these compounds be directly detected in the brains of animals with objectively confirmed evidence of tinnitus?

TABLE 1
fnsys-06-00009-t001.gif
Table 1. Candidate compensatory mechanisms potentially responsible for compensatory "restoration" of central function following sensory organ damage.

Over the past decade animal models have contributed significantly to the neuroscience of tinnitus (Roberts et al., 2010). While diverse, all animal models assume that the sensation of tinnitus results from relatively primitive alterations in central auditory processing. In animals, tinnitus is induced by manipulations, such as high-level sound exposure, that commonly produce tinnitus in humans. The presence of tinnitus can be revealed by appropriately designed psychophysical procedures (Brozoski and Bauer, 2008). In the present experiment, the brains of rats with psychophysical evidence of sound-induced chronic tinnitus were imaged, and point-resolved proton magnetic resonance spectra (1H-MRS) were obtained from tissue volumes localized to areas in the auditory pathway that have been identified in one or more studies as potentially involved in mediating tinnitus. Analyzed areas included the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN), the inferior colliculus (IC), the medial geniculate body (MGB) or auditory thalamus, and primary auditory cortex (A1).

Recent technical improvements have enabled the acquisition of well-resolved spectra from small tissue volumes. Point-resolved spectroscopy (PRESS) can be used to optimize signal strength from small volumes, while ultra-short echo times can be used to reduce signal multiplets thereby improving peak dispersion and the resolution of complex spectra such as those obtained from brain tissue. Insertion of asymmetric variable power RF pulses into the probe sequence can be used to reduce the otherwise intrusive water peak that obscures signals above 3 ppm (Mlynarik et al., 2008). Separation of spectral lines and extraction of weak signals can be further enhanced by using very high-level magnetic fields and additionally through the use of a tunable pickup coil (Odintsov, 2011).

In the present investigation spectra were obtained from brain volumes of interest (VOI). The brain spectra were calibrated against spectra obtained from glass phantoms, filled with known concentrations of GABA and Glu, and comparable in size to the VOI. GABA and Glu were selected for analysis because they are, respectively, the major inhibitory and excitatory neurotransmitters in the auditory pathway; their concentrations are at detectible levels in small brain volumes, and they have been identified as potentially playing a significant role in tinnitus.

Materials and Methods
Subjects
Twenty adult male Long–Evans rats (Harlan, Indianapolis, IN), 90 days old at the start of the experiment, were individually housed and maintained at 25°C with a 12/12 h reversed light/dark schedule. Ten months prior to spectroscopy 16 of the animals participated in a study examining the effect of supplementary dietary taurine on tinnitus. The experimental protocol was approved by the Laboratory Animal Care and Use Committee of Southern Illinois University School of Medicine and the University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign.

Tinnitus Induction
Half of the animals (n = 10) were unilaterally exposed once to band limited noise for 1 h. These subjects will be referred to as "exposed." The remaining animals (n = 10) were treated identically, but unexposed. They will be referred to as "unexposed." Sound exposure parameters were identical to those reported previously to produce tinnitus in rats (Bauer and Brozoski, 2001; Brozoski et al., 2012). All subjects, exposed and unexposed, were anesthetized to an areflexive state, using either an isoflurane/O2 mixture (Aerrane, Baxter Healthcare Corp., Deerfield, IL, USA), or a ketamine/xylazine mixture (24.6 and 3 mg/kg, respectively; ketamine, Ketaset, Fort Dodge Animal Health, Fort Dodge, IA; xylazine, Anased, Lloyd Laboratories, Shenandoah, IA). For the exposed animals, peak level was 116 dB (SPL) centered at 16 kHz, with the noise band falling to ambient level at 8 kHz and 24 kHz. The sound was delivered monaurally using a speaker driver (FT17H, Fostex, Tokyo, Japan) in a custom enclosure funneling the sound to a flexible tube that fit snugly into the auditory canal. Sound levels were calibrated using a Brüel and Kjaer (Norcross, GA, USA) Pulse sound measurement system (Pulse 13 software), equipped with a 3560C high-frequency module, and a 4138 pressure-field microphone (Brüel and Kjaer) coupled to the transducer using rubber tubing with the internal dimensions of an adult rat external auditory canal. All sound levels reported in the present experiment are unweighted pressure levels re 20 μ Pa.

Hearing Levels
Hearing thresholds were determined immediately before and after exposure (for exposed rats) using auditory brainstem-evoked potentials (ABR). They were also obtained at the conclusion of tinnitus testing. ABR measurements were obtained using either a TDT System 3 Real Time Signal Processing System running BioSig32 and SigGen (Tucker Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL, USA), or an IHS Smart EP System, running IHS High Frequency Software (v. 2.33) and using IHS high frequency transducers (HFT9911–20–0035, Intelligent Hearing Systems, Miami, FL). Evoked responses were differentially recorded from a subcutaneous vertex needle electrode referenced to an electrode at the occiput. Evoked responses for 10 msec epochs following stimulus onset were amplified × 100,000, bandpass filtered (100–3000 Hz), and averaged for 512 repetitions of each frequency-intensity level combination. Digitized records of the evoked responses (40 μ sec resolution) were exported to Excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA) for analysis and threshold determination. Hearing thresholds for each ear were defined by the lowest stimulus level that produced statistically distinct and visually distinct evoked waveforms, defined as the maximum peak-to-peak deflection within the 10 msec window following stimulus onset. Custom applications written for Excel were used for analysis.

Tinnitus Assessment
Tinnitus was measured after sound exposure, using a behavioral assay shown to be sensitive to tinnitus in rats, and described in detail elsewhere (Bauer and Brozoski, 2001; Brozoski et al., 2012). Briefly, an operant conditioned-suppression procedure was used to determine the animal's perception of test tones and silent periods embedded in an ambient of low level (60 dB, SPL) broad-band noise (BBN). The key feature of the procedure was that animals were required to discriminate between the presence and absence of sound when tested with a variety of sounds varying in composition, frequency, and level. While the features of tinnitus in rats (and humans) cannot be directly known, it is certain that tinnitus cannot sound like silence. The animals were tested daily in commercial operant test chambers (Lafayette Instruments, Mod. 80001, Lafayette, IN, USA) equipped with lid-mounted speakers. Speaker-off periods (i.e., silence) had a special significance because lever pressing for food during the silent periods led to a foot shock at the end of that period. The behavior of interest was lever pressing during randomly presented test sounds (1 min duration) that substituted for some of the speaker-off presentations (also 1 min duration). Assessment sessions consisted of 10 randomly inserted, non-contiguous, presentations. Two of the 10 were always silent (i.e., speaker-off) periods. The remaining eight presentations were of a randomly selected tone or noise, with different levels in each presentation. Lever pressing was quantified using a relative rate measure, the suppression ratio (R). R was determined as a running measure for successive 1 min segments of each session using the formula R = B/(A + B), where A was the number of lever presses in the preceding 1 min segment and B the number of lever presses in the current 1 min segment. R can vary between 0 and 1. A value of 0 is attained when lever pressing in the current minute is 0, a value of 0.5 when lever pressing in the current minute is equal to that of the previous minute and a value of 1 when lever pressing in the previous minute is zero. R provided a running index of behavior, in 1 min segments, and enabled a quantitative comparison between subjects as well as unbiased compilation of group data. R is a useful index of perceptual performance in that it is very sensitive to short-term behavioral effects, such as those produced by sensory events, but it is very insensitive to gradual behavioral effects, such as those produced by changes in motivational status, for example, satiation. In the context of the present procedure, it was expected that exposed rats with tinnitus would have lower R values than unexposed rats, when tested with stimuli that resembled their tinnitus. All test sessions were 60 min in duration. Further details of the psychophysical procedure appear in an open-source document (Brozoski et al., 2012).

Exposed and unexposed rats were treated identically and tested in parallel. Individual animal and group discrimination functions were derived from the final 3–5 sessions of each test series, where performance variability was minimal. Test stimuli were BBN, and 8, 10, 16, 20, 24, and 32 kHz tones. Each stimulus was tested across a range of levels from low to high audibility. Evidence of tinnitus was determined by the divergence of group discrimination functions. For subjects with tinnitus, test stimuli that resembled their tinnitus served as a signal for response (lever press) suppression. In contrast, for unexposed subjects without tinnitus, the signal for suppression was silence. Test stimuli with sensory features resembling tinnitus, therefore, produced greater suppression (i.e., fewer lever presses) in subjects with tinnitus. Previous research (Bauer and Brozoski, 2001) has shown that Long–Evans adult rats, unilaterally exposed to high-level band-limited noise centered at 16 kHz, show evidence of tinnitus in a range between 10 and 30 kHz.

Spectroscopy: Preparation
Prior to spectroscopy, each animal in its home cage, was placed in a double-wall sound insulated booth for 20 h without food but with freely available water. The objective was to equate the baseline acoustic environment for all subjects, and to reduce the acoustic ambient to a low-level. The sound floor in this environment across the audible frequency range for rats was less than 10 dB (SPL). Ten minutes prior to spectroscopy, animals were pretreated i.p. with 10 mg/kg 3-mercaptopropionic acid (product M5801, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) to arrest post-mortem GABA inflation (van der Heyden and Korf, 1978). Immediately before acquisition, animals were given a lethal dose of anesthetic (Euthasol, Virbac, Ft. Worth, TX), decapitated, the mandible removed, and excess muscle tissue dissected away from the skull. The head was placed in a polyethylene holder along with a 1 mm diameter glass capillary filled with CuSO4 (3 mM). The image phantom of the capillary indexed the left hemisphere, making VOI laterality unambiguous.

Spectroscopy: Calibration
Prior to spectrum acquisition, glass vessels 2 mm in diameter were prepared with either 10 mM of GABA or 10 mM of Glu dissolved in sterile normal saline. Initial attempts to include one or more calibration vessels along with the brain during spectrographic data acquisition distorted the field, making shimming impossible. Therefore, calibration spectra were obtained either before or after acquisition of brain spectra. Initial calibrations were done with GABA and Glu in combination with compounds, such as creatine, that were expected to produce unwanted spectral lines potentially obscuring those of GABA and Glu. Spectrograms of the mixtures were used to identify GABA and Glu peaks, from among their multiplets, that provided the clearest separation from background. Calibration and peak selection were further refined using arrays of phantoms distributed in the field of view as shown in Figure 1. Taking into account all calibration results, GABA levels were determined using the peak at 2.18 ppm and Glu using the peak at 3.68 ppm (Figure 2).

FIGURE 1
fnsys-06-00009-g001.gif
Figure 1. Calibration of GABA spectra using an array of phantoms, each containing 10 mM GABA in sterile normal saline, distributed across the imaging field of view. Using multiple phantoms arrayed throughout the field of view illustrates optimized field shimming and selection of the least-variant spectral peak, from among the multiplets at each location, used to quantify the compound of interest (in this example, GABA).

FIGURE 2
fnsys-06-00009-g002.gif
Figure 2. Calibration spectra of GABA and Glu, obtained using the same scanning parameters (see Methods for details) used to capture VOI spectra. The peak, from among the multiplets, used to quantify GABA and Glu is shown by the pointer.

Spectroscopy: Data Acquisition
At the conclusion of behavioral testing, the rats were individually imaged and volume-localized 1H-MRS spectra were determined. Spectra were obtained using a vertical bore Varian Unity/Inova 600 mHz NMR spectrometer with a 14.1 T magnet. To reduce the displacement error of microvolume localized 1H-MRS, caused by large chemical shifts in the strong magnetic field, a hybrid short-pulse sequence, obtained from R. Gruetter (Mlynarik et al., 2008), was used. The pulse sequence was optimized for signal acquisition in the spectral band containing the neurochemicals of interest, i.e., GABA and Glu. Signals were further optimized using a tunable pickup coil (Odintsov, 2011).

For each animal, an initial MRI brain scan was used to locate the VOI for 1H-MRS. Contiguous transverse (i.e., coronal) slices, 0.5 mm thick (26 μ m planar resolution), were obtained, extending 13 mm caudally from Bregma (26 slices total). VOI for 1H-MRS were determined within the auditory pathway as indicated in Table 2. The VOI for each auditory region was kept constant and its placement was made as standard as possible using image reference points. 1H-MRS spectra were obtained bilaterally in the order indicated in Table 2, with acquisition parameters optimized for each VOI. When shimming, the water peak for all spectra was held constant at 4.7 ppm, and its line width minimized (typically 30–45 Hz). Three hundred scan repetitions were adequate to resolve all spectra, with the exception of the DCN (Table 2).

TABLE 2
fnsys-06-00009-t002.gif
Table 2. 1H-MRS acquisition parameters.

Spectroscopy: Data Analysis
Spectra, as TIFF images, were imported into Image J (ver. 1.44p, http://imagej.nih.gov/ij). Peaks in closest approximation to the calibration peaks for GABA and Glu, were outlined, and the area under each curve (AUC) for each was determined. AUCs were expressed in spectrum baseline (i.e., curve lower bound) units in order to correct for image gain. AUC values were copied to an Excel (2007, Microsoft, Redmond, WA) spreadsheet for analysis. The AUC of each neurochemical was converted to concentration level (mM), taking into account VOI volume, calibration vessel volume, known calibration concentration, and calibration AUC, as indicated in Equation 1.

fnsys-06-00009-i001.gif


Pair-wise comparisons were made between exposed and unexposed VOI. Reported significance levels were derived from independent t-tests.

Results
Evidence of Tinnitus
Significant tinnitus was evident in the exposed rats approximately four months after exposure. The protocol in the present experiment (exposure before psychophysical training and testing), reveals tinnitus as a downshift in discrimination functions. This happens because the sensory correlate of speaker-off periods, i.e., tinnitus in exposed animals, is the conditioned stimulus for response suppression. Therefore, stimuli resembling the speaker-off sensation suppress responding. Frequency-specific suppression was evident in both individual (Figure 3) as well as group data (Figure 4), between 16 and 24 kHz. Individual subjects showed some variation in the frequency range of their suppression, from rather narrow (Figure 3A) to broad (Figure 3B). This may reflect the tonality of their tinnitus. In addition, some individuals showed evidence of tinnitus plus hyperacusis. Nevertheless, all of the exposed rats showed some evidence of tinnitus.

FIGURE 3
fnsys-06-00009-g003.gif
Figure 3. Examples of tinnitus in individual rats.(A) A rat with focal narrow-band tinnitus in the vicinity of 20 kHz, and (B) A rat with rather broad-band tinnitus localized between 10 and 24 kHz. Error bars show the standard error of the mean of the unexposed group. Relative rate of lever pressing, as indicated by the suppression ratio (see Text), is shown on the y-axis, and test stimulus across stimulus levels (dB, SPL) is shown on the x-axis.

FIGURE 4
fnsys-06-00009-g004.gif
Figure 4. Frequency-specific group evidence of tinnitus. The statistics in each panel represent a comparison of exposed and unexposed groups for stimulus levels above the OFF setting. As in Figure 3, relative lever pressing is shown on the y-axis and test stimulus across levels is shown on the x-axis. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.

GABA and Glu Levels
1H-MRS data were collected at the conclusion of psychophysical testing. Data from six exposed and five unexposed rats were included in the spectral analysis. Data from seven initial rats were used to adjust and optimize experimental parameters and were not included in the final analysis. Data from two rats were discarded when pituitary tumors were detected. Mean age at the time of spectroscopy was 19.8 and 17.8 months, respectively, for the exposed and unexposed animals. The mean interval between exposure and spectroscopy was 16.2 months. Acquisition of spectra from one animal, along with attendant field shimming, required approximately 7 h to complete. One session was run per day. Spectra for the four auditory areas of interest, DCN, IC, MGB, and A1, are summarized in Figures 58. Each figure depicts selection of the VOI in the top panel, a representative individual spectrum from that VOI in the center panel, and estimated GABA and Glu average levels for exposed and control animals in the bottom panel. GABA and Glu estimated mM levels are reported separately for each hemisphere, ipsilateral, and contralateral, with respect to sound exposure. Decreased GABA concentration was evident only in the MGB, with the greatest decrease, relative to unexposed controls, obtained in the contralateral MGB (Figure 7; p = 0.033). GABA levels in the exposed MGB were 0.38 mM (±0.18) contralateral and 1.62 mM (±0.64) ipsilateral, as compared to unexposed levels of 2.77 mM (±1.04) contralateral and 2.57 mM (±1.39) ipsilateral. Error (±) is the standard error of the mean. Therefore, a GABA decrease, and potential loss of GABA-mediated inhibition, was most evident in the auditory thalamus in the direct pathway from the exposed ear, i.e., the contralateral MGB. Small GABA increases may have been present bilaterally in A1 (Figure 8) and in the contralateral DCN (Figure 5) however, these differences were not statistically significant.

FIGURE 5
fnsys-06-00009-g005.gif
Figure 5. GABA and Glu levels (mM/ml) in the dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN) of exposed and unexposed rats. Top panel: size and location of the VOI; Mid panel: a typical individual 1H-MRS; Lower panel: group average GABA and Glu levels. Error bars show the standard error of the mean. Asterisk next to the axis label indicates the direct pathway with respect to the trauma-exposed ear. Asterisk next to the data bar indicates statistical significance (p ≤ 0.05).

FIGURE 6
fnsys-06-00009-g006.gif
Figure 6. GABA and Glu levels (mM/ml) in the inferior colliculus (IC) of exposed and unexposed rats. No significant differences were obtained between exposed and unexposed animals in the IC. Graphic parameters as in Figure 5.

FIGURE 7
fnsys-06-00009-g007.gif
Figure 7. GABA and Glu levels (mM/ml) in the medial geniculate body (MGB) of exposed and unexposed rats. GABA and Glu concentrations were significantly lower in the contralateral MGB of exposed animals. Graphic parameters as in Figure 5.

FIGURE 8
fnsys-06-00009-g008.gif
Figure 8. GABA and Glu levels (mM/ml) in primary auditory cortex (A1) of exposed and unexposed rats. Although differences between exposed and unexposed animals were not statistically significant, significance levels may have been masked by measurement variability stemming from field anisotropy rather that neurochemical variability. Graphic parameters as in Figure 5.

Glu level variation, both within and between treatment groups, was higher than GABA variation. Despite the variation, Glu was significantly (p = 0.039) elevated in the exposed ipsilateral DCN, 18.77 mM (±9.76) while unexposed was 12.36 mM (±2.32). Contralateral DCN Glu levels were not significantly different in exposed and unexposed rats. The contralateral exposed level was 6.52 mM (±5.00) while unexposed was 3.08 mM (±2.09) (Figure 5). Exposed ipsilateral and contralateral Glu levels were elevated in A1 with respect to unexposed controls, but not significantly so (Figure 8; p = 0.082, ipsilateral; p = 0.145, contralateral). However, significance was negatively impacted by high variation in spectra obtained from A1. Field shimming was difficult for A1, and this suggested that the measurement variation may have been a consequence of field anisotropy near the periphery, rather than intrinsic tissue factors. A1 exposed ipsilateral Glu was 3.12 mM (±1.64) while unexposed was 1.32 mM (±0.54). Exposed contralateral A1 Glu was 2.60 mM (±1.55) while unexposed was 0.81 mM (±0.59).

In the MGB Glu was significantly decreased contralaterally (1.24, ± 0.67 vs. 4.55, ± 1.13; p = 0.029) and non-signficantly increased ipsilaterally (4.36, ± 1.91 vs. 2.59, ± 1.11; p = 0.448). In summary, these results suggest enhanced Glu-mediated excitation in the DCN, and potentially in A1, although variation in cortical levels obscured significance. In the IC, exposed vs. unexposed differences in both GABA and Glu were small (Figure 6).

Hearing Thresholds
The sound exposure used to produce tinnitus was unilateral. This was done to preserve free-field hearing thresholds, a requirement for psychophysical testing. Immediately after exposure (peak sound level at 116 dB, SPL), exposed ear hearing thresholds, as indicated by ABR, were elevated approximately 60 dB at 20 kHz (Figure 9, top panel). Thresholds for unexposed ears were not affected by the exposure. At the conclusion of psychophysical testing, and prior to MRS data collection, ABR thresholds for exposed ears returned to normal levels (Figure 9, bottom panel). Recovery of threshold sensitivity is typical for this level of exposure (Bauer and Brozoski, 2001) in Long Evans rats.

FIGURE 9
fnsys-06-00009-g009.gif
Figure 9. Auditory brainstem response thresholds for exposed and unexposed rats. Top panel: thresholds before and immediately after acoustic exposure. Significant threshold elevation was evident only in the exposed ear. Bottom panel: thresholds prior to spectroscopy show normal levels in both groups. Error bars show the standard error of the mean.

Discussion
Volume level vs. Neurotransmitter Level
The present results indicated that there was a GABA decrease in the MGB and Glu increase in both the DCN and A1, accompanying chronic tinnitus in a rat model. This would support the hypothesis that region-specific loss of inhibition and increased excitatory neurotransmission, underpins chronic acoustically induced tinnitus. However, support of the neurotransmitter hypothesis must be tempered when taking into account limitations of the current method. Neurochemical concentrations were determined ex vivo using point-resolved 1H-MRS and a 14.1 T magnet. Spectra were effectively resolved from volumes down to 4.5 mm3 (i.e., the DCN volume). But the reported levels reflect concentrations of the compounds of interest throughout the tissue volume from which spectra were obtained. Using an MRI scan of each brain, care was taken to place the measurement volume entirely within each area of interest. Within each VOI, however, 1H-MRS cannot distinguish concentrations in specific sub-VOI compartments, for example, within neurotransmitter vesicles. In addition to their role as neurotransmitters, GABA and Glu also participate in general metabolic functions, for example as components of the tricarboxylic acid cycle GABA shunt. A further complexity is that GABA is derived from Glu by enzymatic decarboxylation. Therefore, the concentration levels reported in the present research cannot be construed as neurotransmitter levels. However, in the present experiment comparisons were made between exposed and unexposed rats, treated identically except for one high-level sound exposure. On average the sound exposure occurred 16 months prior to spectroscopy. It is unlikely that general metabolic consequences of the exposure would linger to influence the results. A reasonable interpretation would be that between-treatment-group variation in GABA and Glu levels reflects functional changes heavily dependent upon neurotransmitter variation (Stagg et al., 2011b).

Reported Levels, Calibration, and Comparison to other Estimates
The GABA and Glu levels reported in the present study were concentrations determined in reference to external calibration standards (see Methods). In contrast, many 1H-MRS studies report relative levels with respect to an internal standard, which is another signal within the spectrum, such as that of N-acetyl aspartate (Stagg et al., 2011a), or water (Puts et al., 2011). When using an internal standard it must be assumed that the standard compound remains constant in concentration across conditions and VOI. The advantage of using an internal standard is that variation in local measurement conditions, for example shimming parameters, does not affect the reported values. The primary disadvantage of using an internal standard is that concentration of the standard compound might not be constant across individuals and conditions, thus affecting the reported ratios. A second disadvantage is that only relative level ratios can be reported, actual concentration levels remain unknown. When using an external standard, as in the present research, physical concentration levels are known and internal standard variation is irrelevant. However, when using an external standard, accuracy is dependent upon the calibration method. The external method described in the present study attempted to establish both realistic and stable calibration conditions (Figures 1 and 2). Calibration spectra were determined from phantoms similar in volume to the VOI and filled with realistic concentrations of GABA and Glu. Calibration spectra were also determined proximate in time to the brain spectra. Unfortunately attempts to simultaneously calibrate by including phantoms along with the brain at the time of data acquisition were not fruitful. Calibration phantoms included along with the brain produced field distortions that made shimming impossible. Also, placing phantoms alongside the brain put the phantoms in the field periphery where locally poor isotropy yielded calibration spectra of low quality.

Glu levels reported in the present study varied between 0.81 and 18.77 mM, while GABA levels varied between 0.09 and 2.77 mM, depending upon the VOI. These ex vivo levels are comparable to levels reported by others using similar 1H-MRS methods. Mlynarik et al. (Mlynarik et al., 2008) reported whole rat brain Glu levels of 10.3 (±8%) and GABA levels of 1.5 (±13%) mM/kg. In comparison to contemporary studies using traditional quantitative analytic methods, Glu levels reported in the present study were within range while GABA levels were somewhat higher. For example a recent report by (Zhu et al., 2011) using current-generation high performance liquid chromatography and whole brain homogenates, reported adult rat brain GABA levels of about 0.5 (±25%) mM/mg and Glu levels of 10 (±10%) mM/mg. The GABA discrepancy between the present study and that of (Zhu et al., 2011) may derive from differences between the methods, liquid chromatography depending upon solvent extraction from whole brain homogenates, and 1H-MRS depending on spectra derived from in situ tissue in the auditory pathway. It may also be that in the present study there was some GABA inflation, despite the use of 3-mercaptopropionic acid to arrest post-mortem elevation. A GABA inflation error, if present, should have affected exposed and unexposed rats equally since GABA (and Glu) levels were determined using the same procedural sequence for all animals (Table 2).

GABA Decrease in the MGB
It has been hypothesized that a loss of inhibition in the central auditory pathway underpins chronic tinnitus (Holt et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2011; Brozoski et al., 2012). Indirect evidence supporting the hypothesis comes from pharmacological studies where systemically administered GABA agonists have been shown to decrease or eliminate behavioral evidence of tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2007b, 2010; Yang et al., 2011). The hypothesis is also indirectly supported by studies showing increased levels of spontaneous neural activity in animals with evidence of tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2002; Kaltenbach and Godfrey, 2008; Middleton et al., 2011). The role of the MGB, i.e., the auditory thalamus, in tinnitus is currently not well defined. Recent imaging evidence suggests that altered thalamic circuits may gate abnormal brainstem spontaneous activity to the cortex and limbic structures in tinnitus patients (Rauschecker et al., 2010). The MGB is an obligatory relay for ascending auditory information. Loss of inhibition in the MGB could increase the impact of spontaneous brainstem activity on higher processes, and induce the perception of sound in the absence of peripheral stimulation. Understanding the function of GABA in the MGB is currently evolving. The density of post-synaptic GABA-A receptors is quite low (Halonen et al., 2009). However, GABA-A receptors containing a δ subunit in substitution for the more common γ subunit, have a high density in the MGB compared to other forebrain areas (Halonen et al., 2009; Richardson et al., 2011). δ-subunit-containing GABA-A receptors appear to be primarily extra-synaptic and are responsible for mediating tonic inhibition (Richardson et al., 2011). This suggests that ambient extracellular GABA is important for regulating the general balance of inhibition/excitation in the MGB. Tinnitus may be mediated by loss of inhibitory volume transmission in the MGB mediated by down-regulation of extracellular GABA. Although the density of GABAergic interneurons in the rat MGB is apparently very low (Winer and Larue, 1996), glia have been shown to be a significant source of GABA in other brain areas (Olah et al., 2009). The loss of MGB GABA reported in the present experiment may reflect decreased extracellular-levels as well as lowered levels in glia. The net decrease in inhibition would increase rostral excitation.

Llinas et al. (2005) present a cortico-thalamic network model to account for tinnitus and other neuro-psychiatric disorders. In their model, cortical pyramidal cell feedback to the auditory thalamus, both indirectly, via the thalamic reticular nucleus, and directly, enables low-frequency thalmo-cortical oscillation. In this model, loss of afferent inhibitory input to the thalamus is hypothesized to lead to "edge effect" enhancement of low-frequency (8 Hz) oscillation, the result being the sensation of tinnitus. Interestingly the model also predicts increased low-frequency oscillation, and presumably, associated tinnitus, in the presence of "protracted hyperpolarization of thalamic cells" (Llinas et al., 2005). Therefore, in the Llinas model, elevated GABA intrinsic to the MGB would be expected to produce chronic tinnitus. The present results, however, show just the opposite, with MGB GABA decreased in rats with tinnitus. Further research with cellular-level resolution will be required to answer more detailed questions concerning the role of GABA in specific circuits.

Glu Increase in DCN
In addition to lowered MGB GABA levels, the present research also showed moderately elevated Glu levels in the DCN. Increased spontaneous neural activity in the DCN is associated with tinnitus in rodent models (Brozoski et al., 2002; Kaltenbach et al., 2004), while altered tonotopic organization of the auditory cortex has also been associated with tinnitus in rats (Yang et al., 2011). In fact there may be widespread increases in central gain functions following sensory loss, such as that produced by traumatic high-level sound exposure (Norena, 2011). Adaptive alterations in brain function are primarily mediated by plastic glutamatergic synapses via long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) (Zhuo, 2009; Lovinger, 2010; Kullmann and Lamsa, 2011). While LTP and LTD, as characterized in vitro, have durations of only an hour or so, it is clear that the same processes are involved in chronic changes that have been reported in adult rat A1 in the context of specific pharmacological and acoustic exposures (Hogsden et al., 2011). Increased Glu levels in the DCN and A1 found in the present study, in rats with chronic tinnitus, may well reflect very long-term increases in excitatory neurotransmission. Both LTP and LTD have been identified in DCN local networks (Tzounopoulos et al., 2007). Furthermore, Glu associated alterations of cochlear nucleus function have been identified following cochlear damage (Zeng et al., 2009).

Null Effect in the Inferior Colliculus (IC)
Given considerable evidence that the IC is involved in chronic tinnitus in animals (Brozoski et al., 2007a; Holt et al., 2010) and humans (Gu et al., 2010), it was somewhat surprising that neither GABA nor Glu levels in the IC were different in exposed versus unexposed animals (Figure 6). One explanation may be that average neural activity level was only slightly elevated in the IC of exposed animals. While significantly involved in tinnitus, the neural correlate in the IC may involve alterations in patterns of neural activity, such as infrequent bursts of high-frequency spikes and decreased inter-spike interval variance (Bauer et al., 2008), rather than gross level increases. As such, volume GABA and Glu alterations may not be evident. It also might be noted that studies reporting loss of IC GABA receptors after cochlear damage, also report permanent threshold elevations (Dong et al., 2010). In the present study hearing thresholds were essentially normal at the time of tinnitus testing and spectroscopy (Figure 9, bottom panel).

Implications for Tinnitus Therapy
There is no generally effective therapeutic intervention for tinnitus in current clinical application. In contrast, in animal models of tinnitus, several GABA agonists have been found to effectively attenuate or eliminate evidence of tinnitus (Brozoski et al., 2007b, 2010; Yang et al., 2011). Interestingly, while peripheral blockade of cochlear glutamatergic NMDA receptors has been shown to attenuate acoustic-trauma-induced tinnitus in a rat model (Guitton and Dudai, 2007), administration of the systemic NMDA antagonist ketamine, was not effective (Yang et al., 2011). Clinically, neither GABA agonists, for example the many available benzodiazepine derivatives, nor NMDA antagonists, have been shown to be effective in well-controlled trials. The present research suggests that a combined approach, targeting both decreased GABA function and elevated Glu function, may be more fruitful.

Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments
Supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, #1R01DC009669–01.

References
Bauer, C. A., and Brozoski, T. J. (2001). Assessing tinnitus and prospective tinnitus therapeutics using a psychophysical animal model. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 2, 54–64.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bauer, C. A., Turner, J. G., Caspary, D. M., Myers, K. S., and Brozoski, T. J. (2008). Tinnitus and inferior colliculus activity in chinchillas related to three distinct patterns of cochlear trauma. J. Neurosci. Res. 86, 2564–2578.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brozoski, T. J., and Bauer, C. A. (2008). Learning about tinnitus from an animal model. Semin. Hear. 29, 242–258.

Brozoski, T. J., Bauer, C. A., and Caspary, D. M. (2002). Elevated fusiform cell activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of chinchillas with psychophysical evidence of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 22, 2383–2390.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Brozoski, T. J., Caspary, D. M., Bauer, C. A., and Richardson, B. D. (2010). The effect of supplemental dietary taurine on tinnitus and auditory discrimination in an animal model. Hear. Res. 270, 71–80.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brozoski, T. J., Ciobanu, L., and Bauer, C. A. (2007a). Central neural activity in rats with tinnitus evaluated with manganese-enhanced magnetic resonance imaging (MEMRI). Hear. Res. 228, 168–179.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brozoski, T. J., Spires, T. J., and Bauer, C. A. (2007b). Vigabatrin, a GABA transaminase inhibitor, reversibly eliminates tinnitus in an animal model. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 8, 105–118.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brozoski, T. J., Wisner, K. W., Sybert, L. T., and Bauer, C. A. (2012). Bilateral dorsal cochlear nucleus lesions prevent acoustic-trauma induced tinnitus in an animal model. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 13, 55–66.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dong, S., Rodger, J., Mulders, W. H., and Robertson, D. (2010). Tonotopic changes in GABA receptor expression in guinea pig inferior colliculus after partial unilateral hearing loss. Brain Res. 1342, 24–32.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gu, J. W., Halpin, C. F., Nam, E. C., Levine, R. A., and Melcher, J. R. (2010). Tinnitus, diminished sound-level tolerance, and elevated auditory activity in humans with clinically normal hearing sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 3361–3370.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guitton, M. J., and Dudai, Y. (2007). Blockade of cochlear NMDA receptors prevents long-term tinnitus during a brief consolidation window after acoustic trauma. Neural Plast. 2007, 80904.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halonen, L. M., Sinkkonen, S. T., Chandra, D., Homanics, G. E., and Korpi, E. R. (2009). Brain regional distribution of GABA(A) receptors exhibiting atypical GABA agonism: roles of receptor subunits. Neurochem. Int. 55, 389–396.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hogsden, J. L., Rosen, L. G., and Dringenberg, H. C. (2011). Pharmacological and deprivation-induced reinstatement of juvenile-like long-term potentiation in the primary auditory cortex of adult rats. Neuroscience 186, 208–219.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Holt, A. G., Bissig, D., Mirza, N., Rajah, G., and Berkowitz, B. (2010). Evidence of key tinnitus-related brain regions documented by a unique combination of manganese-enhanced MRI and acoustic startle reflex testing. PLoS One 5, e14260. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014260

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaltenbach, J. A., and Godfrey, D. A. (2008). Dorsal cochlear nucleus hyperactivity and tinnitus: are they related? Am. J. Audiol. 17, S148–S161.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaltenbach, J. A., Zacharek, M. A., Zhang, J., and Frederick, S. (2004). Activity in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of hamsters previously tested for tinnitus following intense tone exposure. Neurosci. Lett. 355, 121–125.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kullmann, D. M., and Lamsa, K. P. (2011). LTP and LTD in cortical GABAergic interneurons: emerging rules and roles. Neuropharmacology 60, 712–719.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Llinas, R., Urbano, F. J., Leznik, E., Ramirez, R. R., and van Marle, H. J. (2005). Rhythmic and dysrhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA systems and the edge effect. Trends Neurosci. 28, 325–333.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lovinger, D. M. (2010). Neurotransmitter roles in synaptic modulation, plasticity and learning in the dorsal striatum. Neuropharmacology 58, 951–961.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Middleton, J. W., Kiritani, T., Pedersen, C., Turner, J. G., Shepherd, G. M., and Tzounopoulos, T. (2011). Mice with behavioral evidence of tinnitus exhibit dorsal cochlear nucleus hyperactivity because of decreased GABAergic inhibition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7601–7606.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mlynarik, V., Cudalbu, C., Xin, L., and Gruetter, R. (2008). 1H NMR spectroscopy of rat brain in vivo at 14.1Tesla: improvements in quantification of the neurochemical profile. J. Magn. Reson. 194, 163–168.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muhr, P., and Rosenhall, U. (2011). The influence of military service on auditory health and the efficacy of a Hearing Conservation Program. Noise Health 13, 320–327.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nondahl, D. M., Cruickshanks, K. J., Wiley, T. L., Klein, R., Klein, B. E., and Tweed, T. S. (2002). Prevalence and 5-year incidence of tinnitus among older adults: the epidemiology of hearing loss study. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 13, 323–331.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Norena, A. J. (2011). An integrative model of tinnitus based on a central gain controlling neural sensitivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1089–1109.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Odintsov, B. (2011). Tunable Radio-Frequency Coil. USA patent application 61081954. November 1, 2011.

Olah, S., Fule, M., Komlosi, G., Varga, C., Baldi, R., Barzo, P., and Tamas, G. (2009). Regulation of cortical microcircuits by unitary GABA-mediated volume transmission. Nature 461, 1278–1281.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Puts, N. A., Edden, R. A., Evans, C. J., Mcglone, F., and Mcgonigle, D. J. (2011). Regionally specific human GABA concentration correlates with tactile discrimination thresholds. J. Neurosci. 31, 16556–16560.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., Leaver, A. M., and Muhlau, M. (2010). Tuning out the noise: limbic-auditory interactions in tinnitus. Neuron 66, 819–826.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Richardson, B. D., Ling, L. L., Uteshev, V. V., and Caspary, D. M. (2011). Extrasynaptic GABA(A) receptors and tonic inhibition in rat auditory thalamus. PLoS One 6, e16508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016508

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roberts, L. E., Eggermont, J. J., Caspary, D. M., Shore, S. E., Melcher, J. R., and Kaltenbach, J. A. (2010). Ringing ears: the neuroscience of tinnitus. J. Neurosci. 30, 14972–14979.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Bachtiar, V., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2011a). The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr. Biol. 21, 480–484.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Bachtiar, V., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2011b). What are we measuring with GABA magnetic resonance spectroscopy? Commun. Integr. Biol. 4, 573–575.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tzounopoulos, T., Rubio, M. E., Keen, J. E., and Trussell, L. O. (2007). Coactivation of pre- and postsynaptic signaling mechanisms determines cell-specific spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Neuron 54, 291–301.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van der Heyden, J. A., and Korf, J. (1978). Regional levels of GABA in the brain: rapid semiautomated assay and prevention of postmortem increase by 3-mercapto-propionic acid. J. Neurochem. 31, 197–203.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Wang, H., Brozoski, T. J., and Caspary, D. M. (2011). Inhibitory neurotransmission in animal models of tinnitus: maladaptive plasticity. Hear. Res. 279, 111–117.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winer, J. A., and Larue, D. T. (1996). Evolution of GABAergic circuitry in the mammalian medial geniculate body. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 3083–3087.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yang, S., Weiner, B. D., Zhang, L. S., Cho, S. J., and Bao, S. (2011). Homeostatic plasticity drives tinnitus perception in an animal model. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 14974–14979.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeng, C., Nannapaneni, N., Zhou, J., Hughes, L. F., and Shore, S. (2009). Cochlear damage changes the distribution of vesicular glutamate transporters associated with auditory and nonauditory inputs to the cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 29, 4210–4217.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhu, K. Y., Fu, Q., Leung, K. W., Wong, Z. C., Choi, R. C., and Tsim, K. W. (2011). The establishment of a sensitive method in determining different neurotransmitters simultaneously in rat brains by using liquid chromatography-electrospray tandem mass spectrometry. J. Chromatogr. B Analyt. Technol. Biomed. Life Sci. 879, 737–742.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhuo, M. (2009). Plasticity of NMDA receptor NR2B subunit in memory and chronic pain. Mol. Brain 2, 4.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: tinnitus animal model, 1H-MRS, GABA, glutamate, dorsal cochlear nucleus, inferior colliculus, medial geniculate, primary auditory cortex

Citation: Brozoski T, Odintsov B and Bauer C (2012) Gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamic acid levels in the auditory pathway of rats with chronic tinnitus: a direct determination using high resolution point-resolved proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS). Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:9. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00009

Received: 21 December 2011; Accepted: 08 February 2012;
Published online: 24 February 2012.

Edited by:

Jos J. Eggermont, University of Calgary Medicine, USA
Reviewed by:

Thanos Tzounopoulos, University of Pittsburgh, USA
Helmy Mulders, University of Western Australia, Australia
Copyright: © 2012 Brozoski, Odintsov and Bauer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited.

*Correspondence: Thomas Brozoski, Division of Otolaryngology, Southern Illinois University School of Medicine, 801 N. Rutledge Street, Rm 3205, PO Box 19629, Springfield, IL, USA. e-mail: tbrozoski@siumed.edu

Write a comment...
Add
  • PDF
  • XML
  • Export Citation
1,596 total views

View Article Impact
FRONTIERS
0
0
0
SHARE ON
0 2 0 0 247
TABLE OF CONTENTS
 

Log in or register to get the full forum benefits!

Register

Register on Tinnitus Talk for free!

Register Now